The Effect of Low-Error and Full-Error Learning on Bimanual Coordination In Patients with High-Functioning Autism Disorder

Document Type : Original Articles

Authors

1 PhD Candidate of Motor Learning, Department of Sport Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, School of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran

2 Associate Professor, Department of Sport Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, School of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran

3 General Practitioner, Gonbad Kavus Health Center, Gonbad Kavus, Iran

10.22122/jrrs.v14i2.3149

Abstract

Introduction: In many sports movements, and even regular movements, the simultaneous use of both hands results in the successful implementation of motor skills. Considering many problems faced by the patient with autism in learning skills, and their impact on social behaviors, the purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of low-error and full-error learning on bimanual coordination in patients with high-functioning autism disorder.Materials and Methods: This was a semi-experimental research with pretest-posttest design, and control group. 24 children with high-functioning autism disorder, resulted from with the help of pretest Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire (SSRQ) were selected using convenience sampling method, and were homogeneously divided into three groups of low-error learning, full-error learning, and control. The dual coordination pretest was done with the help of the bimanual coordination instrument of the Omide Farda Institute (Tehran, Iran). Then, the training groups did practicing bimanual coordination skills on the same machine in six sessions of 30 minutes. At the end of the sixth session, the posttest of bimanual coordination skills was performed in all groups. Two days later, a retention test was taken from the training groups. The results were analyzed using one way and repeated measures analysis of variance.Results: There were no significant differences between the groups in pretest (P = 0.643). While in posttest, a significant difference was observed between the practice and control group (P = 0.001). Moreover, there was a significant difference between low-error and full-error learning groups (P = 0.001). The retention test, which was performed after two days of rest, revealed a decrease in performance of full-error group.Conclusion: According to the results, it is concluded that these individuals are able to improve their bimanual coordination skills, and low-error learning is not only useful in learning, but also has more sustainability than full-error learning. Therefore, it is recommended to use a low-error learning method to teach bimanual coordination skills in patients with high-functioning autism disorder.

Keywords

  1. Strunk J, Leisen M, Schubert C. Using a multidisciplinary approach with children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. J Interprof Educ Pract 2017; 8: 60-8.
  2. Chen Z, Kuo LJ. Language and literacy development among children with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Dev Disord 2017; 3(3): 1-4.
  3. Schiffer RB, Rao SM, Fogel BS. Neuropsychiatry. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2003.
  4. Vogan VM, Francis KE, Morgan BR, Smith ML, Taylor MJ. Load matters: Neural correlates of verbal working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord 2018; 10(1): 19.
  5. Chmielewski WX, Beste C. Action control processes in autism spectrum disorder--insights from a neurobiological and neuroanatomical perspective. Prog Neurobiol 2015; 124: 49-83.
  6. Garcia-Villamisar D, Sala SD. Dual-task performance in adults with autism. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2002; 7(1): 63-74.
  7. Gooijers J, Swinnen SP. Interactions between brain structure and behavior: The corpus callosum and bimanual coordination. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 43: 1-19.
  8. Isenhower RW, Marsh KL, Richardson MJ, Helt M, Schmidt RC, Fein D. Rhythmic bimanual coordination is impaired in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 2012; 6(1): 25-31.
  9. Zwart FS, Vissers CTWM, Kessels RPC, Maes JHR. Implicit learning seems to come naturally for children with autism, but not for children with specific language impairment: Evidence from behavioral and ERP data. Autism Res 2018; 11(7): 1050-61.
  10. Hirsch SB. ERP Correlates of procedural learning: designing a task for children with autism [BA Thesis]. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University; 2010.
  11. Boucher J, Anns S. Memory, learning and language in autism spectrum disorder. Autism and Developmental Language Impairments 2018; 3: 2396941517742078.
  12. Schmidt RA. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review 1975; 82(4): 225-60.
  13. Reber AS, Walkenfeld FF, Hernstadt R. Implicit and explicit learning: individual differences and IQ. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1991; 17(5): 888-96.
  14. Kriete T, Noelle DC. Implicit learning deficits in autism: A neurocomputational account. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society 2009; 2009 Jul 29-Aug 1; Amsterdam, Netherlands. p. 899-904.
  15. Wegrzyn AK. Motor learning in children with an autism spectrum disorder [MA Thesis]. Columbia, MO: University of Missouri-Columbia; 2013.
  16. Gordon B, Stark S. Procedural learning of a visual sequence in individuals with autism. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl 2007; 22(1): 14-22.
  17. Brown J. An analysis of functional differences in implicit learning [PhD Thesis]. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge; 2010.
  18. Barnes KA, Howard JH, Jr., Howard DV, Gilotty L, Kenworthy L, Gaillard WD, et al. Intact implicit learning of spatial context and temporal sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology 2008; 22(5): 563-70.
  19. Nemeth D, Janacsek K. The dynamics of implicit skill consolidation in young and elderly adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011; 66(1): 15-22.
  20. Wulf G, Shea CH, Whitacre CA. Physical-guidance benefits in learning a complex motor skill. J Mot Behav 1998; 30(4): 367-80.
  21. Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. Implicit motor learning, reinvestment and movement disruption: what you don't know won't hurt you. In: Williams AM, Hodges NJ, editors. Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory, and practice. London, UK: Routledge; 2004. p. 207-28.
  22. Gallahue DL, Ozmun JC. Understanding motor development: Infants, children, adolescents, adults. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2000.
  23. Hasan Barani F, Abdoli B, Farsi A. Effect of errorless and errorful learning on performance kinematic parameters in a throwing task: A pilot study. J Res Rehabil Sci 2014; 9(6): 978-90.
  24. Jalili F, Bahrami H, Nejati V. Comparing diagnostic ability of basic emotional states in children with high performance autism disorder with normal peers. Zahedan J Res Med Sci 2012; 14(2): 39-44.
  25. Abedanzadeh R, Abdoli B, Farsi A. The effect of sensory feedback on the transition of the relative phase in bimanual coordination task in old adults. J Res Rehabil Sci 2015; 11(1): 42-50. [In Persian].
  26. Ramezanzade H, Doraneh Kord M. The effect of attention focus in errorless and errorful practice conditions on performance and learning of dart throwing skill. Journal of Motor Learning and Movement 2018; 10(1): 121-38. [In Persian].
  27. Pascualvaca DM, Fantie BD, Papageorgiou M, Mirsky AF. Attentional capacities in children with autism: Is there a general deficit in shifting focus? J Autism Dev Disord 1998; 28(6): 467-78.
  28. Delacato CH. The diagnosis and treatment of speech and reading problems. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; 1967.
  29. Chiou SC, Chang EC. Bimanual coordination learning with different augmented feedback modalities and information types. PLoS One 2016; 11(2): e0149221.
  30. Maslovat D, Brunke KM, Chua R, Franks IM. Feedback effects on learning a novel bimanual coordination pattern: support for the guidance hypothesis. J Mot Behav 2009; 41(1): 45-54.
  31. Green D, Chambers ME, Sugden DA. Does subtype of developmental coordination disorder count: is there a differential effect on outcome following intervention? Hum Mov Sci 2008; 27(2): 363-82.
  32. Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Landa RJ, Denckla MB. Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6(7): 752-9.
  33. Grainger C, Williams DM, Lind SE. Judgment of learning accuracy in high-functioning adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2016; 46(11): 3570-82.
  34. Capio CM, Poolton JM, Sit CH, Holmstrom M, Masters RS. Reducing errors benefits the field-based learning of a fundamental movement skill in children. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2013; 23(2): 181-8.
  35. Ghamari A, Mohamadi J, Mohamadi M. The effect of errorless and errorfull practice on learning and transfer of dart throwing skill in adolescents with intellectual disabilities. Motor Behavior 2015; 7(21): 111-26. [In Persian].
  36. Maxwell JP, Masters RS, Kerr E, Weedon E. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. Q J Exp Psychol A 2001; 54(4): 1049-68.
  37. Zhu FF, Poolton JM, Wilson MR, Maxwell JP, Masters RS. Neural co-activation as a yardstick of implicit motor learning and the propensity for conscious control of movement. Biol Psychol 2011; 87(1): 66-73.
  • Receive Date: 11 October 2018
  • Revise Date: 26 April 2024
  • Accept Date: 22 May 2022